1 Introduction
The World Wide Web is enormous, free, immediately available, and largely linguistic. As we discover, on ever more fronts, that language analysis and generation benefit from big data, so it becomes appealing to use the Web as a data source. The question, then, is how. (Kilgarriff, 2007)
Linguists have been using text data from the web in published work since at least Grefenstette (1999). The appeal, as Kilgarriff (2007) notes, is the low cost of entry: Most researchers have quick and easy access to a search engine—even quicker since 2007, following the rise of internet-enabled mobile devices.
The cost of entry for speech research on the web remains considerably higher, even as the publication of new speech data on the web accelerates, due to platforms like iTunes and YouTube and to infrastructure with greater bandwidth and storage capacity.
The problem, of course, is that search engines search text, not speech. Unless a transcription exists, speech data on the web are effectively invisible. And even when a transcription does exist, it may not be time-indexed. This problem is shared by those who publish speech to the web, and who want to maximize public exposure to their content. For these content producers, there is an obvious commercial incentive to create searchable, time-indexed transcriptions.
At this time, speech researchers do not have access to anything approaching the power and scope of a Google text-search. Nonetheless, the quantity of transcribed, time-aligned speech online is significant and growing. In this study, we present a proof-of-concept for harvesting and analyzing speech data from the web. We leveraged two websites (Everyzing.com and play.it) which indexed radio podcasts with transcriptions obtained with automatic speech recognition (ASR). While the transcriptions varied in quality at the sentence level, accuracy typically exceeded 50% the level of short, common word sequences (Howell & Rooth, 2009).
Our greatest motivation in using web-harvested speech is the dramatic expansion and diversification of the empirical base it offers linguistic theory. Data collected through personal introspection or elicitation in a university or commercial laboratory represent only a small part of the diversity of human speech. Web-harvested speech, because it is naturally-occurring, reflects a more diverse set of speakers and social contexts. Traditional, curated corpora are static (usually by design) and out-dated (because of the time and effort required to assemble and maintain them). The web, by contrast, is dynamic and evolving.
Naturally, speech data from the web cannot replace existing forms of data. Kilgarriff (2007) cautioned researchers on the challenges and pitfalls of using textual data from the web. In the case of speech data, one must proceed with even greater caution, since the text itself, particularly if generated by ASR, introduces additional biases. With this in mind, we implement an approach for validating results obtained from web data by using machine learning and speech data elicited in the laboratory.
We use our approach to investigate the phenomenon of focus sensitivity in English, in which the interpretation of particular grammatical constructions are sensitive to the location of prosodic prominence. Formal semantic theories of focus make clear predictions for the location of focus in a given discourse context and we want to test these predictions by measuring acoustic prominence in naturally-occurring speech.
Unlike the case of introspective or laboratory data, we lack control over the discourse context in naturally-occurring speech, and it may also be challenging to recover the discourse context. For this proof-of-concept study, then, we investigated a focus-sensitive construction in which the relevant discourse antecedent is always explicit. This is the comparative clause than I did, where as we explain below, the location of prominence is predictable from a property of the main clause of the comparative sentence.
We test semantic predictions by building an acoustic classifier. Our methodology uses an explicit machine-learning classification model to evaluate the predictions of a semantic/pragmatic theory of the distribution of focus in a specific syntactic-lexical context, and to investigate the signal features that are involved in marking focus in that context. The methodology is computational and has a certain complexity, but manageable because it takes advantage of well-established classification models and implementations of them in R. In the interest of making it easier to apply the computational method to similar problems, we have distributed code and datasets for the experiments (Howell, 2016).
In addition to evaluating the semantic theory, it is also possible to ask which sets of acoustic measurements contribute to successful classification. We compare the performance of classifiers which use measures of F0 against classifiers which use other, non-F0 measures. And we also compare classifiers which use un-normalized, ‘paradigmatic’ measures against classifiers using ‘syntagmatic’ measures which have been normalized within the utterance.
Finally, we also compare the performance of the machine learning classifiers to that of human listeners, and we test whether the same acoustic measures contribute to human listener performance.
The paper has the following organization. The rest of the introduction elaborates the semantics and pragmatics of focus sensitivity and the phonetics and phonology of prosodic prominence. Section 2 describes the methods of data collection for the laboratory-elicited and web-harvested speech, while Section 3 details the machine learning classification, including the algorithms used and how they are evaluated. In Sections 4 and 5 we report on classification using web data and lab data, respectively. In Section 6, we provide a comparison of logistic regression models; and in Section 7, we report on human listener classification. The conclusion is presented in Section 8. Replication data, including acoustic measurements, scripts and speech recordings, to the extent possible, are published online at the Harvard Dataverse (Howell, 2016).
1.1 The semantics of focus sensitivity
In English, prosody is used to mark certain parts of an utterance as salient in the discourse. For instance, the speaker A in (1) makes it salient that someone ate the sushi, but at the time of B’s utterance it is not yet salient that Sara ate the sushi. We say that Sara is ‘focused’ in (1B) and ate the sushi is not focused or ‘given.’
(1) | A: You ate the sushi. |
B: No, Sara ate the sushi. |
In ‘anaphoric’ or ‘givenness’ theories of focus, we understand the relationship between an utterance and the discourse as a kind of anaphora. Roughly, reduced prominence on ate the sushi in (1B) is licensed by the earlier sequence ate the sushi in (1A).
Discourse anaphors need not be explicit, however. Suppose we are at a Japanese restaurant and order a plate of sushi. You leave for a few minutes and return to find your partner sitting in front of a dirty, empty plate. That someone ate sushi (whether true or not) is now salient in this context, and the utterance in (2) is therefore felicitous, even without an explicit discourse antecedent.
(2) | In the presence of a dirty, empty plate… |
B: Sara ate the sushi. |
Rooth (1992) and Schwarzschild (1999) offer two well known formalisms of focus anaphoricity, the former emphasizing contrastive focus and the latter emphasizing givenness/newness. Both accounts posit a kind of focus skeleton, a semantic object with variables replacing the focused phrases, e.g., ‘X ate the sushi.’ Rooth’s ‘focus semantic value’ is the set of propositions obtained by replacing the focused phrase with alternatives of the same type; Schwarzschild achieves a similar effect by existentially quantifying over focused phrases.1
Thus, from the utterance [Sara]F ate the sushi, Rooth would derive a focus semantic value such as {‘Juan ate the sushi,’ ‘The server ate the sushi,’ ‘The woman at the next table at the sushi,’ … }. Schwarzschild would derive an existentially quantified proposition ‘Someone ate the sushi.’
Focus is licensed if the focus skeleton stands in a particular relation to a discourse antecedent. For Rooth, the antecedent must be an element of the focus semantic value. For Schwarzschild, the antecedent must entail the existentially quantified proposition. Although more work comparing the two formalisms is required, they are largely equivalent with respect to focus anaphoricity (cf. Rooth, 2016). For the following discussion, we’ll assume a relation of entailment holds, following Schwarzschild.
Comparative clauses have the useful property of always occurring with an explicit antecedent: The main clause. Suppose the comparative clause than I did in (3a) is interpreted as ‘I stayed to some degree long.’ With focus on the subject I, we derive an existentially quantified proposition ‘Someone stayed to some degree long,’ which is entailed by the main clause antecedent ‘He stayed to some degree long.’2
Similarly, we derive an existentially quantified proposition ‘I like that song some degree at some time’ in (3b), which is entailed by the main clause antecedent ‘I like that song some degree at the present time.’ In (3c), we derive an existentially quantified proposition ‘I understand some degree little at some time,’ which is entailed by the main clause antecedent ‘I understand some degree little today.’
(3) | (a) | Subject focus (class ‘s’) |
He stayed longer than [I]F did | ||
antecedent: He stayed x long | ||
(b) | Non-subject focus (class ‘ns’) | |
I like that song a lot more than I [did]F | ||
antecedent: I like that song x much | ||
(c) | Non-subject focus (class ‘ns’) | |
I understand less today than I did [yesterday]F | ||
antecedent: I understand x little |
As a proxy generalization3 for focus anaphoricity, we will say that when reference varies in the subject position between the main and than-clauses as in (3a), the subject pronoun I in the than-clause is semantically focused. When reference is constant in the subject position as in (3b) and (3c), semantic focus occurs instead on did or on a following adverbial. We can refer to this generalization as the co-reference criterion (4).
(4) | Co-reference criterion for focus in comparative clauses |
If the subjects of the main and comparative clauses have different referents, the token belongs to class ‘s’ (subject focus); | |
Else, the token belongs to class ‘ns’ (non-subject focus). |
With the co-reference criterion, we have an independent way of classifying the comparatives that does not involve prosody.
Together with an interface principle that relates semantic focus to prosodic prominence, theories of focus anaphoricity make testable predictions for the location of prosodic prominence in comparative clauses. A naïve interface principle states simply that a focused constituent is prosodically prominent. One reason for the naïvety of this principle is the non-trivial computation of ‘focus constituent.’ For example, there is a large literature on focus projection (e.g., Breen et al. 2010; Drubig, 1994, 2003; Gussenhoven, 1992; Jacobs, 1999; Selkirk, 1995; Winkler, 1996) concerned with prominence within large focus constituents (cf. I love [cheese]F vs. I [love cheese]F), arguing against a naïve principle. We set aside this issue here, since the focused constituent at issue in our datasets consists of a single element, namely the pronoun I.
1.2 The prosody of focus sensitivity
As described in Section 2, we extracted more than 300 acoustic measurements from utterances of than I did. In building acoustic classifiers, we do not attempt to make an exhaustive comparison of different combinations of these 309. Nor do these 309 measurements exhaust the possible ways of measuring utterances of this short string. We do, however, consider two ways of grouping the measurements which bear on long-standing issues in the phonetic and phonological study of prosodic prominence.
The first grouping separates syntagmatic measurements (for example, those which relate I and did in the same utterance) from paradigmatic measurements (for example a measurement from I alone). In the last half century, phonologists studying how we produce and perceive prosodic prominence and semanticists studying how we use prosodic prominence to make discourse coherent have advanced their understandings using two ostensibly opposite conceptions of prosodic prominence. Phonologists have argued that prominence should be understood as primarily relational or syntagmatic, e.g., a word or syllable is prominent only with respect to an adjacent word or syllable; semanticists have operated under the tacit assumption that prominence is essentially absolute or paradigmatic: e.g., a word or syllable simply is or isn’t prominent.
(5) | … [ α ]F β … paradigmatic comparison |
↕ | |
… α β … | |
(6) | … [ α ]F β … syntagmatic comparison |
↔ |
Although linguists from many theoretical traditions have noted the syntagmatic nature of prosody (e.g., Jakobson et al. 1952; Ladefoged, 1975; Lehiste, 1970; Saussure, 1967[1916]), the relational nature of prominence was explicitly codified in the theory of metrical phonology (e.g., Giegerich, 1985; Halle & Vergnaud, 1987; Hayes, 1981; Liberman, 1975; Liberman & Prince, 1977; Prince, 1983; Selkirk, 1984), which views prominence, particularly stress, as hierarchically organized rhythmic structure.
In contrast, there is a tradition among semanticists and syntactians to use capitalization, italics or other typographical conventions to indicate prominence, tacitly assuming a paradigmatic comparison. One also finds this view represented in phonetic alphabets, such as the International Phonetic Alphabet, and in early generative theories of prominence (Chomsky & Halle, 1968). More recently, several semantic accounts have attempted to model the semantics after the syntagmatic phonological accounts, evaluating focus or givenness as a relation between pairs of adjacent constituents (e.g., Rooth, 2009, 2015; Wagner, 2005, 2006; Williams, 1997).
The second grouping separates measures of F0 from all other measures. The work of Fry (1955, 1958) long ago dispelled the myth that prominence was realized primarily by loudness. Since then, however, the scientific literature on acoustic prominence has been dominated by discussion of fundamental frequency and pitch. Kochanski (2006) reported that, in one sample, articles about F0 outnumbered articles investigating other prosodic cues by nearly 5 to 1. Yet different lines of research have pointed to the robustness of non-F0 measures. Work in laboratory phonetics and phonology has identified non-F0 cues of accent in speech production (e.g., Campbell & Beckman, 1997; Cho, 2006; De Jong, 1991; Ladefoged, 1967; Ladefoged & Loeb, 2002; Lehiste, 1970) and in the acoustics of speech (e.g., Beckman & Pierrehumbert, 1986; Lehiste, 1970).
In the domain of phonology, work in the autosegmental tradition (e.g., Bruce, 1977; Goldsmith, 1976; Leben, 1973; Liberman, 1975; Pierrehumbert, 1980) motivated a distinction between pitch accent and stress. This leads to the question of which category—pitch accent or stress—is the primary correlate of semantic focus: In the derivationally-oriented terminology of Selkirk (1984), whether focus is ‘stress-first’ or ‘accent-first.’
Semanticists remained largely unconcerned with the debate, apart from intensive investigation of the licensing configuration for putatively ‘accentless’ second occurrence focus (e.g., Beaver & Clark, 2008; Kadmon, 2001; Partee, 1991; Rooth, 1996). Experimental studies of this phenomenon (e.g., Bartels, 2004; Beaver et al., 2007; Bishop, 2008; Howell, 2011) confirmed that significant pitch cues of prominence were indeed absent. However, other acoustic measures of prominence related to stress, such as duration and intensity, were present in small but statistically significant amounts.
Experimental evidence also suggests at least three categorical levels of prominence. Beckman and Edwards (1994) studied the articulation of the syllable pa in three contexts, which we will refer to as phrase accented, word accented, and unaccented: The first syllable of papa (7a), the first syllable of papa in (7b) and the second syllable of papa in (7b), respectively. The phrase-accented syllable carries a pitch accent and has an unreduced vowel; the prosodic word-accented syllable is postnuclear and has an unreduced vowel; the unaccented syllable has a reduced vowel.
(7) | a. | [Was her mama a problem about the wedding?] |
Her Papa posed a problem. | ||
b. | [Did his dad pose a problem as far as their getting married?] | |
Her papa posed a problem. |
This categorical distinction was first proposed by Bolinger (1958, 1981) and Vanderslice and Ladefoged (1972) (Gussenhoven 2004: 20; see also Halliday 1967). This influential distinction between phrase accenting and word accenting is fundamental to the ToBI annotation framework (Beckman & Ayers, 1994).
Beckman and Edwards observe that the contrast between the accented syllable and the unaccented syllable is particularly robust for vowel duration and the degree and speed of jaw opening movement. We can infer that vowel reduction is also correlated with less extreme formant movement. Although we do not use them here, measures of spectral balance and post-focal compression have also been implicated in distinguishing between levels of stress (e.g., Sluijter & van Heuven, 1996; Xu et al., 2004).
2 Methods of data collection
2.1 Web harvested data
We collected two different web-harvested corpora of utterances containing than I did, using a methodology detailed in Howell and Rooth (2009). A set of standard UNIX tools (e.g., curl, cutmp3, awk, bash, make) replicates user interaction with an external search engine. The search engine, provided by RAMP (formerly Everyzing), uses automatic speech recognition to index speech and identify possible utterances of a word sequence, in our case than I did. The first corpus (web1) was collected using their search interface at Everyzing.com; the second corpus (web2) was collected using their search interface at play.it. The Everyzing interface searched content from a variety of content providers, but predominantly radio stations, including WEEI, WNYC, KPBS, WRKO, NPR and the White Rose Society. The play.it interface searched content from various member stations of CBS radio.4
Retrieval efficacy varies by dataset, but Howell and Rooth found that roughly 50% of purported tokens were true, unique and readable. Manual filtering was required. Dataset web1 contained 90 true tokens of than I did: 45 tokens with subject focus (‘s’) and 45 tokens with non-subject focus (‘ns’). Dataset web2 contained 127 true tokens: 62 tokens with subject focus and 65 tokens with non-subject focus.
The antecedent and comparative clause in each token was manually transcribed into English prose. From this transcription, the tokens were manually categorized into one of the two focus categories, according to the co-reference criterion (cf. 4). Although this semantic classification was performed by humans, the task did not require special expertise or training beyond identifying and comparing grammatical subjects of the two clauses.
2.2 Laboratory-elicited data
2.2.1 Stimuli
A total of 16 written stimuli were constructed, modeled after attested examples in the web-harvested corpora. Eight of the stimuli contained an ordinary, first occurrence focus (e.g., 8) and the other eight contained both a first occurrence focus and a repeated, second occurrence focus. The conditions for second occurrence focus were created by contrasting an adjective or verb (e.g., longer in 9) or by contrasting a degree modifier (e.g., lot in 10).
(8) | He saw the situation differently than I did. | FOF stimulus (subject focus) |
(9) | You worked harder than I did, | FOF stimulus (subject focus) |
and you worked longer than I did. | SOF stimulus (subject focus) | |
(10) | I think Tom said it a little better than I did. | FOF stimulus (subject focus) |
In fact, he said it a lot better than I did. | SOF stimulus (subject focus) |
Among the FOF-only stimuli, half were statements (e.g., 11) and half were wh-questions (e.g., 12).
- (11)
- There were a lot of photographers who would shoot more than I did.
- (12)
- Why do I have more energy today than I did the day before?
Each experimental condition was balanced for semantic focus condition: Half of the tokens had subject focus and half had non-subject focus. The full set of stimuli are given in Appendix B. In order to limit the scope of this paper, we leave the SOF examples for future analysis. Henceforth, any mention of laboratory data (lab) will refer only to the FOF examples.
2.3 Recording
Participants were recorded in a sound-attenuated room. Twenty-seven individuals participated, although one participant’s speech failed to be recorded, leaving a total of 26 participants. Participants were paid.
The stimuli were presented on a computer screen using a set of MATLAB scripts written by Michael Wagner for conducting prosody experiments. In addition to the 16 target sentences, participants also read 18 filler sentences.
No additional context was provided to participants outside of what appears in Appendix B. We choose the comparative construction in order to avoid the challenges of ensuring that each participant used the same discourse antecedent. Since the main clause provides the explicit discourse antecedent for the comparative clause, theory predicts that additional context will be unnecessary for the purpose of conditioning focus.
After reading the text aloud, participants were asked to rate the naturalness of the written stimuli on a scale from 1 (very natural) to 5 (very awkward). The mean rating for the individual stimuli ranged from 1.72 to 3.08; the overall mean was 2.35, suggesting that the stimuli were reasonably naturalistic.
Nineteen tokens were discarded due to speaker disfluencies, such as false starts, hesitations, or utterances that did not match the written stimuli, leaving 397.
2.4 Segmentation
The extraction of acoustic information required annotation at the phonetic level. For each utterance of than I did, the following phonetic segments were annotated: V1, the vowel [æ] of than; N1, the nasal [n] of than; V2, the diphthong [aɪ] of I; C3, the stop closure and burst of the initial [d] in did; and V3, the vowel [ɪ] of did.
The web-harvested data were labeled manually by the experimenters or by research assistants trained for the task. For segmentation criteria, we used oral and nasal constriction landmarks in the spectrogram and waveform: Change in amplitude between vowels and the nasal and oral stops, and the high frequency burst of oral stop releases (cf. Turk et al., 2006).
The laboratory-elicited data were, in addition, automatically forced-aligned using a set of Python scripts that interface with the Hidden Markov Model Toolkit (HTK) (Gorman et al., 2011). Since the manually-annotated laboratory data did not result in improved classification, we report only on the forced-aligned laboratory data. Alignment failed on 3 files for a total of 394 tokens.
2.5 Acoustic extraction
A total of 309 acoustic measures were extracted using the scripting function of Praat (Boersma & Weenink, 2017). Phenomena of interest included duration, fundamental frequency (F0), first and second formants (F1 and F2), intensity, amplitude, voice quality, and spectral tilt. Means or extrema were taken for these phenomena, at regular intervals within a vowel or at the time of other extrema. The ratio between I and did were also calculated for many measurements, including duration, F0 and intensity. The full list of measurements is provided with descriptions in Appendix A.
3 Machine learning classification
A traditional acoustic phonetic study examines a handful of variables relative to a large number of observations (small p, large n), allowing application of methods from classical statistics such as ANOVA and logistic regression. Given a set of more than 300 variables and as few as 90 tokens,5 the data in this study may, by contrast, be considered large p, small n (also known as High Dimension Low Sample Size or, henceforth, HDLSS). Accordingly, we pursue statistical methods for high-dimensional data borrowed from fields where HDLSS data are ubiquitous, including biotechnology, medical imaging, astrophysics, finance, and e-commerce. In genetics, for example, one may wish to examine many thousands of genes in a modest number of tissue samples.
First, we apply two machine learning techniques which have been effective in the classification of HDLSS datasets: Linear discriminant analysis (LDA) and support vector machines (SVMs). Second, and in addition, we apply feature selection, using an automated method and a manual, human method. SVMs map linear features into a multidimensional feature space, while feature selection (also known as feature reduction) reduces an apparently high-dimensional structure to a low-dimensional structure.
The machine learning classification and feature selection methods are discussed in Sections 3.1 and 3.2, respectively. Sections 3.3 and 3.4 describe the methods of evaluating classifier performance and the division of data into test sets and training sets.
3.1 Classification algorithms
Two machine learning techniques were used to create predictive models of the data. Linear discriminant analysis (LDA) is a classification framework based on multidimensional Gaussian probability distributions that has been used widely in pattern recognition tasks (Venables & Ripley, 2002). Support vector machines (SVMs) (Boser et al., 1992) are a relatively recent method of supervised classification that have achieved excellent accuracy in tasks such as object recognition (Evgeniou et al., 2000), cancer morphology identification (Mukherjee et al., 1999), and text categorization (Joachims, 1997). In both cases, we begin with training data consisting of vectors of acoustic measurements, divided into an ‘s’ set from tokens with shifting reference in the subject position, and ‘ns’ for constant reference in the subject position. An estimation procedure produces a real-valued objective function h of the linear form (13a). It can be used used to label points in the space with ‘s’ or ‘ns,’ according to the decision rule (13b).
The decision surface for the model is the surface that divides points that are classified as ‘s’ from those classified as ‘ns.’ In a dataset with two dimensions, the decision surface is a line dividing the two-dimensional space, and in general, in a dataset with n features, a hyperplane (i.e., an affine subspace of dimension n – 1). Figures 1 and 2 illustrate decision surfaces in two-dimensional and three-dimensional models drawn from our data.
(13) | (a) | h (x) = w · x + b |
(b) | if h (x)>0 then s else ns |
An LDA model is estimated by fitting a multivariate Gaussian distribution to the data with each label, subject to a constraint of equal co-variance for the two distributions. A Bayes optimal decision rule then results in a linear decision surface. In contrast to an LDA model, which because of the estimation procedure is sensitive to all the training data, the decision plane in an SVM model is sensitive only to a subset of the training data. The plane is positioned in a way that maximizes distance to nearby data points (the support vectors), and includes also a penalty for mis-classified data (Cortes & Vapnik, 1995).
LDA models make assumptions which may not be satisfied by the true distributions for our problem, namely normality of the distributions, and (assuming normality) equality of covariances for the two classes. Poor results may also obtain if the training set is small.6 Furthermore, the LDA classifier has been shown to perform best when the number of attributes is minimized (ideally no greater than 2 attributes for a binary classifier) and when the attributes are not intercorrelated (cf. Brown & Wicker, 2000).7 Our 309 acoustic measurements outnumber the tokens in our datasets, and groups of features are likely to be highly correlated. In the next section, we discuss a method of attribute selection, in order to reduce this number of attributes. In practice, however, it is often possible to obtain good results for an LDA classifier even with small datasets and even with data in violation of the assumptions of normal distribution and homogeneity of covariances (e.g., Klecka, 1980; Lachenbruch, 1975; Stevens, 2002). The implementation of linear discriminant function analysis we use is available in the MASS package (Venables & Ripley, 2002) for the statistical computing environment R (R Development Core Team, 2016). The implementation of SVM we use is available in the libsvm package (Chang & Lin, 2011; Dimitriadou et al., 2009) for R.8
Another feature of SVMs is the possibility mapping of linear attributes into a multi-dimensional feature space. This is done by replacing dot products by a non-linear kernel function.9 This greatly reduces the typical computational complexity of training, at the cost of somewhat increased computational complexity during testing.10 Although the data should be internally scaled for best results, use of a non-linear kernel also avoids the need to transform attributes which may be non-linear (e.g., duration and energy in our data). Many kernel functions have been used successfully in different classification tasks. Hsu et al. (2003) recommend a radial basis function (RBF),11 a non-linear mapping which has been shown to also encompass a linear kernel (Keerthi & Lin, 2003) and which behaves similarly to a sigmoid kernel (Lin & Lin, 2003). Hsu et al. note that the RBF kernel requires only two hyperparameters, while a polynomial kernel, for example, will contain two or more, contributing to model complexity. (All kernels contain at least one hyperparameter C, cost or constant, which sets the penalization for a datum occurring on the wrong side of the margin.) At the same time, Hsu et al. also suggest that the results of a linear kernel may be comparable with those of an RBF kernel in situations where the number of attributes to be mapped is greater than the number of data instances. This situation obtains for those of our classifiers which use the unfiltered set of 309 attributes and are applied to datasets of 90 and 127 (viz. the web-harvested datasets). We therefore consider classifiers using both RBF kernels in addition to linear ones. Figure 2 shows the curved decision boundary obtained in a three-dimensional model with an RBF kernel.
Estimating LDA and SVM classifiers requires datasets without missing values. Algorithms in Praat and other acoustic analysis software have a notoriously difficult time extracting values such as F0 in the absence of regular, periodic voicing. A dataset with missing values was therefore unavoidable, and many values were undefined. Typically, a dataset with less than 5% missing data is considered manageable, while more than 5–10% missing data may bias subsequent statistical analysis and can require sophisticated methods of data imputation. All of our datasets fell within these acceptable rates of missingness. The web-harvested datasets web1 and web2 had missing rates of 0.6% and 0.8%, respectively. The laboratory-elicited dataset had a missing rate of 2.5%.
Many different kinds of data imputation exist. Single imputation methods replace all missing data with the same value, such as –1, 0 or 1, or the mean or median of the variable. One disadvantage of single imputation is that it fails to model the variability of the underlying data. Multiple imputation methods use algorithms to impute a particular value for each data point using information from observations without missing data. Some common methods of multiple imputation include hot- and cold-deck imputation and k-nearest neighbor imputation. We experimented informally with several of these methods of imputation and none resulted in noticeable differences in classifier performance. Leaving in-depth study of imputation for future research, we chose to use mean imputation on all of the datasets.
Optimizing the value of hyperparameters is often recommended. In this study, we were able to achieve robustly performing classifiers with the default settings ( and C = 1). We therefore leave the contribution of tuning to future investigation.
3.2 Redundant features and feature selection
In building a classifier, one may be concerned simply with the classification task itself: Developing and improving the ability of a particular decision function to generalize from one set of data (a training set) to another (a test set). We may call this the ‘functional measure’ (cf. Cristianini & Shawe-Taylor, 2000).
One may also be concerned with how the classification task is achieved and how closely it models real human cognitive ability. We may call this the ‘descriptional measure.’ The relative importance of the functional and descriptional measures typically varies according to the goals of the researcher. Consider the following functionally-oriented view from Cristianinni and Shawe-Taylor.
Shifting our goal to generalisation removes the need to view our hypothesis as a correct representation of the true function. […] In this sense the criterion places no constraints on the size or on the ‘meaning’ of the hypothesis – for the time being these can be considered to be arbitrary. (Cristianini & Shawe-Taylor, 2000: Section 1.2)
Another more descriptionally-oriented researcher concerned primarily with the underlying or ‘true’ function may be wary of even a high-accuracy decision function which incorporates what may seem to be linguistically irrelevant or orthogonal noise in the data.
In practice, however, the functional and descriptional are not mutually exclusive and are, one hopes, mutually informative. One may, for example, apply the functional measure to establish a pattern in the data and apply other methods to understand the contribution of different features in the model. In this study, we want a classifier which accurately predicts a focus category—a functional measure, but we also wish to know which acoustic measures are important for this task—a descriptional measure.
Feature selection is one means of peering into the ‘black box’ of a classifier, and understanding which features are contributing to a model’s generalization accuracy. Pragmatically, feature selection is also sometimes necessary to improve classifier performance. Collinearity in LDA models have been shown to lead to stability problems (e.g., Naes and Mevik 2001). SVMs, despite their promise as a classifier which does not require feature selection, have been shown to improve the generalization accuracy and/or model complexity (and thus computation) for those datasets with redundant and/or irrelevant features. For example, Sarojini et al. (2009) demonstrate improved accuracy for a clinical dataset with a large number of instances (768) and a small number of features (8 prior to feature elimination) while conversely Weston et al. (2001) demonstrate this effect for cancer discrimination in a dataset with a small number of instances (72) and a large number of features (7129 genes). More generally, removing redundant features mitigates the potential for a classifier to be mislead and for overfitting of the model.
Most authors agree that some combination of manual and statistical feature selection techniques may be used, although there is no consensus on the ordering or relative importance of manual or statistical feature selection:
Feature selection should be viewed as a part of the learning process itself, and should be automated as much as possible. On the other hand, it is a somewhat arbitrary step, which reflects our prior expectations on the underlying target function. (Cristianini & Shawe-Taylor, 2000: Chapter 3)
To start, the initial variable list should be logically screened, based on substantive theory, prior research, and reliability of measures, as well as on practical grounds. Next, the list can be statistically screened. (Huberty, 2006: 11)
Of course, an investigator’s professional opinion also can be relied upon when selecting potential discriminator variables. (Brown & Wicker, 2000: 212)
Many statistical methods of feature selection exist. Filter methods select features according to some importance measure independent of the classifier, such as correlation or information gain. Embedded methods incorporate selection into the training process of classification; a set of minimally optimal features for the classification task are identified. As a reviewer notes, features which are pre-selected automatically may however cause an increase in the generalization error rate of a classifier (see for example Barron, 1994). In contrast, wrapper methods use information from a classifier (possibly a different classifier) prior to training and are used to select not just a set of non-redundant features, but all relevant features. With a functional measure in mind, we chose an all-relevant wrapper method known as the Boruta algorithm which is available as an R package (Kursa & Rudnicki, 2010). Briefly, the algorithm generates fake or ‘shadow’ features and iteratively compares the real features against them, using a random forest classifier to compute a significance measure.
In addition to applying the Boruta algorithm to the full set of 309 acoustic measures, we also applied the algorithm to theoretically meaningful subsets: F0-related measures, non-F0-related measures, syntagmatic measures (i.e., ratios between I and did), and paradigmatic measures (i.e., from a single word). Finally, based on a combination of theoretical expectation and trial-and-error, we also selected several feature sets by hand.
Note that because we are considering two different training sets (first the web-harvested dataset web1 and later the laboratory-elicited dataset lab), we apply feature selection independently for the two sets. The results of the Boruta algorithm are detailed in Section 4.1.
The set of acoustic measures used by a machine learning classifier to predict focus will not necessarily correspond to the set of acoustic measures that a human speaker uses to convey focus or to the set of features an individual human listener uses to interpret focus. It is therefore important to investigate the use of acoustic measures in human classification, which we do in Section 7.
3.3 Evaluation of classifier performance
Typically, a classification algorithm generates a model from a set of labeled data (a training set) and this model is then used to predict unseen data without labels (a test set). If the correct labels of the test set are known, we can compare them against the model’s predictions. The proportion of correct labels and the proportion of incorrect labels are known as the generalization accuracy and generalization error, respectively. As a measure of bias–whether the classifier tends to predict one class more accurately than the other–we calculate a balanced error rate (BER), which is an average of the two within-class error rates. We also compare the results against a simple baseline accuracy, which is the proportion of the larger class. The three statistics are given in (14, 15, 16).
- (14)
- (15)
- (16)
In addition to calculating these performance statistics for each classifier, we also wanted some confidence that a classification model wasn’t overfitting the particular training data and that its performance on the test data was not by chance. To assess this, we performed permutation-based validation (cf. Hsing et al. 2003; Jensen, 1992; Molinaro et al., 2005). The class labels of the training set were randomly permuted before training and performance statistics calculated in the usual way. This process was iterated n times. In theory, one may repeat this for all possible permutations, although this strategy is impractical for computational reasons. A large number of iterations (we chose n = 5000) produces a reasonable approximation of the empirical cumulative distribution for the permutation-achieved performance statistics.
In Figure 3, we plot the empirical distribution of permutation-achieved generalization accuracy for a particular classifier. The x-axis represents generalization accuracy; the y-axis represents the cumulative distribution (i.e., the proportion of the permuted data which is less than or equal to the value of x). If the observed accuracy or balanced error falls outside of the 95th or 99th percentile, we say that the observation is statistically significant with a p-value of greater than 0.05 or 0.01, respectively. This provides a confidence measure with which we can reject the null hypothesis that the classifier achieved the observed statistic at random.
We can visualize the significance of multiple classifiers in one figure by plotting the observed and permutation-achieved statistics as single points, as in Figure 4 (cf. Lyons-Weiler et al., 2005). The observed statistic is plotted in black; the permutation achieved statistic at p = 0.05 and p = 0.01 is plotted in green and red, respectively. If the observed statistic falls outside of the permutation achieved statistic, we say that the observed statistic is statistically significant.
For convenience, we plot the accuracy rate and BER on the same figure. An asymmetry between the two, therefore, represents a bias towards one of the two classes.
More extreme permutation-achieved statistics (i.e., greater accuracy or smaller balanced error) reflect more structure in the data. For example, if the acoustic values are randomly distributed with respect to the two focus classes, we expect less variance among the permutation-based statistics and less extreme statistics at p = .01 and p = .05. If the acoustic values are nonrandomly distributed with respect to the classes, we expect more variance and more extreme statistics at p = .01 and p = .05, since some permutations will be more positively and negatively correlated with the acoustic values.
Note that this permutation test does not directly compare one classifier’s performance against another’s. In order to determine this, we performed McNemar’s test (McNemar, 1947; see also Gillick & Cox, 1989), a test for the difference of two proportions which has been used to compare machine learning classifiers. A non-parametric counterpart of the t-test, McNemar’s test compares the null hypothesis that the two classifiers misclassify the same tokens, using a χ2 test for goodness-of-fit. Although it doesn’t measure variability due to choice of training set (e.g., web1 vs. lab), McNemar’s test does exhibit acceptably low Type I error and reasonably high power (Dietterich, 1998). We apply the test to compare a classifier using a subset of features to the related algorithm using the full set of features.
Finally, we wanted to assess the relevance of individual features used in the best performing classifiers. To do this, we compared pairs of logistic regression models–one with and one without the feature of interest–using an ANOVA and chi-squared test of statistical significance.
3.4 Training/test pairs
In order to make comparison manageable, we did not test and train the datasets in all possible combinations. Rather, we used just two datasets for training: The web-harvested dataset web1 and the laboratory-elicited dataset lab. We tested the web-trained classifiers on the remaining web-harvested dataset web2 and on the laboratory-elicited dataset lab (Section 4). We tested the laboratory-trained classifiers on the web-harvested dataset web2 (Section 5). Table 1 summarizes the datasets under consideration.
Name | source | annotation | size (ns/s) | baseline accuracy |
---|---|---|---|---|
web1 | web-harvested:Everyzing | manual | 90 (45/45) | 50.5 |
web2 | web-harvested:play.it | manual | 127 (65/62) | 51.2 |
lab | laboratory-elicited | automated | 394 (193/201) | 51 |
4 Machine classification experiments 1: Web-harvested training data
4.1 Feature selection by algorithm
All-relevant feature selection using the Boruta algorithm applied to the web1 web-harvested dataset produced the feature sets (17, 18, 19, 20, 21). From the full feature set, the algorithm selected a combination of F0, non-F0, syntagmatic, and paradigmatic features that included measures of vowel duration, F0, energy and formant values. No measures of intensity, spectral tilt, jitter, or shimmer were selected.
From the set of exclusively F0 features, Boruta selected measures of the value and timing of F0 means, minima, and maxima, both paradigmatically and syntagmatically.
From the set of exclusively non-F0 features, Boruta selected measures of vowel duration, glottal pulse, intensity, energy, amplitude, and formant values. The duration and formant values were mostly paradigmatic, coming almost exclusively from I; and the values related to loudness (i.e., glottal pulse, intensity, energy and amplitude) were all syntagmatic. The formant values came from the first and second formant of I, both individually and as a ratio, and at intervals from 20 to 70 percent of the vowel duration.
From the set of exclusively paradigmatic features, Boruta selected measures of duration, glottal pulse, minimum and range of F0, and first and second formant values at several different intervals. All of the paradigmatic features were selected from I.
From the set of exclusively syntagmatic features, Boruta selected measures of intensity, amplitude, energy, duration and the value or timing of mean, minimum, maximum and range of F0.
(17) | Features selected by Boruta algorithm from full feature set | ||
duration_V2 | energy_ratio | f2Time60_V2 | |
pulses_V2 | f2Time20_V2 | f1Time70_V2 | |
pulses_ratio | f2Time30_V2 | f1f2Time20_V2 | |
meanf0_ratio | f1Time40_V2 | f1f2Time30_V2 | |
maxf0_ratio | f2Time40_V2 | f1f2Time40_V2 | |
minf0Time_ratio | f1Time50_V2 | f1f2Time50_V2 | |
rangef0_V2 | f2Time50_V2 | f1f2Time60_V2 | |
rangef0_ratio | f1Time60_V2 | ||
(18) | Features selected by Boruta algorithm from set of F0 features | ||
meanf0_ratio | maxf0Time_V3 | rangef0_V2 | |
maxf0_ratio | maxf0Time_ratio | rangef0_ratio | |
minf0_ratio | minf0Time_V2 | ||
maxf0Time_V2 | minf0Time_ratio | ||
(19) | Features selected by Boruta algorithm from set of non-F0 features | ||
duration_V2 | f2Time20_V2 | f1Time70_V2 | |
pulses_V2 | f2Time30_V2 | f1f2Time20_V2 | |
pulses_ratio | f1Time40_V2 | f1f2Time30_V2 | |
maxIntensity_ratio | f2Time40_V2 | f1f2Time40_V2 | |
energy_ratio | f1Time50_V2 | f1f2Time50_V2 | |
amp_ratio | f2Time50_V2 | f1f2Time60_V2 | |
maxf1_V2 | f1Time60_V2 | ||
f1Time20_V2 | f2Time60_V2 | ||
(20) | Features selected by Boruta algorithm from set of syntagmatic features | ||
pulses_ratio | maxf0Time_ratio | energy_ratio | |
meanf0_ratio | minf0Time_ratio | amp_ratio | |
maxf0_ratio | rangef0_ratio | duration_ratio | |
minf0_ratio | maxIntensity_ratio | ||
(21) | Features selected by Boruta algorithm from set of paradigmatic features | ||
duration_V2 | f1Time40_V2 | f1f2Time20_V2 | |
pulses_V2 | f1Time40_V2 | f1f2Time30_V2 | |
minf0Time_V2 | f1Time50_V2 | f1f2Time40_V2 | |
rangef0_V2 | f1Time50_V2 | f1f2Time50_V2 | |
f1f2Time10_V2 | f1Time60_V2 | f1f2Time60_V2 | |
f2Time20_V2 | f1Time60_V2 | ||
f2Time30_V2 | f1Time70_V2 |
4.2 Classifier results
4.2.1 Web-trained, web-tested
In this section we train the classifiers on the web-harvested dataset web1, and test the classifiers on a second web-harvested dataset web2. The results are summarized in Table 2 and Figure 5. Each of the observed classifier accuracies and balanced error rates of the classifiers was statistically significant (p < .05 using a permutation-achieved statistic), with the best performing classifier achieving 92.9% accuracy and 6.5% balanced error.
web1 → web2 | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Feature set | Baseline | SVM (RBF) | SVM (linear) | LDA | |||
1. Full feature set | 51.2 | 83.5 | 13.8 | 79.5 | 18.9 | 78.0 | 21.3 |
2. ‘Best’ features | 51.2 | 83.5 | 16.5 | 88.2 | 11.7 | 86.6 | 12.9 |
3. All F0 features | 51.2 | 80.3 | 19.3 | 71.7 | 28.0 | 73.2 | 26.7 |
4. ‘Best’ F0 features | 51.2 | 75.6 | 24.3 | 76.4 | 23.2 | 75.6 | 24.3 |
5. All non-F0 features | 51.2 | 83.5 | 13.8 | 78.7 | 20.3 | 79.5 | 19.3 |
6. ‘Best’ non-F0 features | 51.2 | 82.7 | 17.3 | 85.8 | 14.1 | 89.9 | 9.9 |
7. All syntagmatic features | 51.2 | 80.3 | 19.6 | 75.6 | 24.1 | 78.0 | 20.1 |
8. ‘Best’ syntagmatic features | 51.2 | 78.7 | 21.2 | 75.6 | 24.4 | 74.8 | 23.9 |
9. All paradigmatic features | 51.2 | 81.9 | 16.6 | 78.7 | 20.0 | 73.2 | 26.6 |
10. ‘Best’ paradigmatic | 51.2 | 82.7 | 17.3 | 87.4 | 12.2 | 89.0 | 10.8 |
11. Experimenter-selected A duration_V2, f1f2Time50_V2, meanf0_ratio, duration_C3 | 51.2 | 91.3 | 7.7 | 92.9 | 6.5 | 92.9 | 6.5 |
12. Experimenter-selected B duration_V2, f1f2Time50_V2, maxf0_ratio, duration_C3 | 51.2 | 92.1 | 7.1 | 92.9 | 6.5 | 92.9 | 6.5 |
13. Experimenter-selected C duration_V2, f1f2Time50_V2, duration_C3 | 51.2 | 89.0 | 9.9 | 91.3 | 7.7 | 90.6 | 8.3 |
The classifiers using the full set of 309 features performed well above the baseline (83.5%, 79.5% and 78.0% with SVM-RBF, SVM-linear and LDA, respectively). Recall, however, that we use feature selection with the intent of removing redundant, potentially misleading features and avoiding overfitting of the model. Most of the classifiers using an automatically selected subset of features failed to achieve a statistically significant improvement (p < .05 using McNemar’s test) over the same algorithm using the full set of features, with the exception of the LDA algorithm using a set of best non-F0 features or a set of best paradigmatic features. In addition, we observed the following numerical tendencies: Classifiers using only non-F0 measures outperformed classifiers using only F0 measures; and classifiers using only paradigmatic measures outperformed classifiers only using syntagmatic measures.
The different algorithms performed competitively, and the classifiers using experimenter-selected features achieved the best overall results, almost all showing a statistically significant improvement over the same algorithm using the full set of features.
4.2.2 Web-trained, lab-tested
Next, we use the same set of web-trained classifiers and test them on laboratory data. The results are summarized in Table 3 and Figure 6.
web1 → lab | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Feature set | Baseline | SVM (RBF) | SVM (linear) | LDA | |||
1. Full feature set | 51.0 | 77.9 | 16.9 | 80.2 | 16.6 | 72.8 | 23.6 |
2. ‘Best’ features | 51.0 | 85.0 | 14.2 | 85.8 | 12.1 | 85.8 | 13.1 |
3. All F0 features | 51.0 | 69.0 | 30.5 | 66.5 | 33.5 | 70.8 | 29.2 |
4. ‘Best’ F0 features | 51.0 | 69.0 | 30.6 | 63.2 | 36.6 | 62.4 | 37.5 |
5. All non-F0 features | 51.0 | 79.7 | 15.3 | 81.0 | 15.7 | 79.7 | 18.8 |
6. ‘Best’ non-F0 features | 51.0 | 83.8 | 14.4 | 83.0 | 14.4 | 81.2 | 15.2 |
7. All syntagmatic features | 51.0 | 75.4 | 22.3 | 70.8 | 28.6 | 64.5 | 35.5 |
8. ‘Best’ syntagmatic features | 51.0 | 73.4 | 25.4 | 67.3 | 32.2 | 64.5 | 35.4 |
9. All paradigmatic features | 51.0 | 78.7 | 16.2 | 73.4 | 20.1 | 78.9 | 19.7 |
10. ‘Best’ paradigmatic | 51.0 | 76.9 | 17.8 | 80.5 | 15.2 | 81.7 | 16.0 |
11. Experimenter-selected A duration_V2, f1f2Time50_V2, meanf0_ratio, duration_C3 | 51.0 | 84.5 | 14.8 | 85.3 | 13.6 | 85.3 | 13.3 |
12. Experimenter-selected B duration_V2, f1f2Time50_V2, maxf0_ratio, duration_C3 | 51.0 | 87.3 | 12.0 | 85.3 | 12.8 | 86.3 | 11.6 |
13. Experimenter-selected C duration_V2, f1f2Time50_V2, duration_C3 | 51.0 | 87.6 | 10.5 | 87.6 | 10.7 | 85.0 | 12.2 |
The observed accuracies and balanced error rates of nearly all of the classifiers were statistically significant (p < .05 using a permutation-achieved statistic), with the best-performing classifier achieving 87.6% accuracy and 10.5% balanced error. The non-significant results came principally from classifiers using only F0 features and only syntagmatic features.
The classifiers using the full set of 309 features performed well above the baseline (77.9%, 80.2% and 72.8% with SVM-RBF, SVM-linear and LDA, respectively). Classifiers using only the automatically selected set of best F0 features demonstrated a statistically significant worse performance (p < .05 using McNemar’s test) over the same algorithm using the full set of features. By contrast, classifiers using only the automatically selected set of best non-F0 features achieved a statistically significant improvement. Similarly, classifiers using only the automatically selected set of syntagmatic features failed to achieve a statistically significant improvement or demonstrated a statistically significant worse performance over the same algorithm using the full set of features, while classifiers using only the automatically selected set of best paradigmatic features achieved a statistically significant improvement.
The different algorithms performed competitively, and the classifiers using experimenter-selected features achieved the best overall results, all showing a statistically significant improvement over the same algorithm using the full set of features.
4.3 Discussion
The performance of classifiers trained on web-harvested data overwhelmingly supports the theoretical predictions for location of prominence in comparative clauses. With few exceptions, classifiers performed well above the baseline, calculated as the percentage of the larger class, and satisfied statistical significance (p < .05), calculated using permutation achieved statistics.
Variation in classifier performance revealed not only the robustness of classifiers using F0 and syntagmatic features, but the robustness of classifiers making use of non-F0 and paradigmatic features. Indeed, a majority of classifiers which used exclusively non-F0 or paradigmatic features achieved statistical significance (p < .05), even on those datasets for which many classifiers using exclusively F0 or syntagmatic features failed to meet statistical significance. It was also the case that neither the classifiers using exclusively F0 features nor the classifiers using exclusively syntagmatic features achieved a statistically significant improvement over the same algorithm using all 309 features. By contrast, classifiers using exclusively non-F0 or paradigmatic features in most cases achieved a performance which is either not significantly different from or significantly better than the same algorithm using all 309 features.
For each dataset, the best performing classifiers used a combination of F0, non-F0, paradigmatic, and syntagmatic features, usually those selected by hand: Duration of I, the first consonant closure duration in did, the F1–F2 differential at the midpoint of I, and the mean or maximum F0 ratio between I and did. Further, the hand-selected classifiers that lacked the latter, syntagmatic F0 measures did not differ significantly from those which included them (at the level of p < 0.05 using McNemar’s test).
Thus, while the syntagmatic and F0 measures are undeniably relevant to the categorization of focus placement in these data (demonstrated also by their selection by the all-relevant Boruta algorithm), the evidence suggests that they may not be necessary. The results of these classification experiments are incompatible with theories of prominence and focus realization which privilege F0 measures and syntagmatic evaluation to the exclusion of other non-F0 and paradigmatically evaluated measures.
We also wish to acknowledge that duration may properly be regarded as syntagmatic, given the effect of speech rate on its interpretation. Among our full set of features we included a syntagmatic measure of duration: The ratio of duration of I to the duration of did. However, unlike the un-normalized measure of duration on I alone, the syntagmatic measure of duration was not selected by the Boruta feature selection algorithm. Anecdotally, the syntagmatic measure of duration was also unhelpful in arriving at an experimenter-selected set of best features.
5 Machine classification experiments 2: Laboratory-elicited training data
5.1 Feature selection by algorithm
All-relevant feature selection using the Boruta algorithm applied to the laboratory-elicited dataset (lab) produced much larger feature sets than when applied to the web-harvested dataset (web1). The feature sets selected are listed in Appendix C. From the full feature set, the algorithm selected 66 different features, which included measures of F0, glottal pulse, jitter, shimmer, intensity, energy, power, first and second formants and duration, and a combination of syntagmatic and paradigmatic measures.
From the set of exclusively F0 features, Boruta selected measures of the value and timing of F0 means, minima, maxima, and range, both paradigmatically and syntagmatically.
From the set of exclusively non-F0 features, Boruta selected 60, which included measures of vowel duration, glottal pulse, jitter, shimmer, intensity, energy, power amplitude and formant values. Both syntagmatic and paradigmatic values were selected, the latter coming from both I and did. The formant values came from the first and second formant of I at intervals from 10 to 80 percent of the vowel duration.
From the set of exclusively syntagmatic features, Boruta selected 15 features, which included measures of amplitude, intensity, energy, power, duration, glottal pulse, and F0.
From the set of exclusively paradigmatic features, Boruta selected 60 features, which included measures of duration, glottal pulse, intensity, power, jitter, shimmer, F0, and first and second formant values, from both I and did.
5.2 Classifier results
5.2.1 Lab-trained, web-tested
In this section, we train classifiers on laboratory data (lab) and test them on web-harvested data (web2) in order to compare results from Section 4. The results are summarized in Table 4 and Figure 7.
lab → web2 | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Feature set | Baseline | SVM (RBF) | SVM (linear) | LDA | |||
1. Full feature set | 51.2 | 77.2 | 22.3 | 77.2 | 22.7 | 72.4 | 27.3 |
2. ‘Best’ features | 51.2 | 78.0 | 19.7 | 78.0 | 22.0 | 77.2 | 22.5 |
3. All F0 features | 51.2 | 67.7 | 32.2 | 65.4 | 34.7 | 60.6 | 38.8 |
4. ‘Best’ F0 features | 51.2 | 70.1 | 29.9 | 66.1 | 33.9 | 59.8 | 39.6 |
5. All non-F0 features | 51.2 | 78.7 | 21.2 | 74.8 | 25.0 | 68.5 | 31.4 |
6. ‘Best’ non-F0 features | 51.2 | 78.0 | 20.2 | 78.0 | 21.4 | 81.1 | 18.2 |
7. All syntagmatic features | 51.2 | 67.7 | 29.1 | 61.4 | 34.7 | 63.0 | 32.5 |
8. ‘Best’ syntagmatic features | 51.2 | 68.5 | 28.5 | 58.3 | 37.3 | 61.4 | 34.0 |
9. All paradigmatic features | 51.2 | 78.0 | 22.0 | 78.7 | 20.9 | 74.0 | 25.7 |
10. ‘Best’ paradigmatic | 51.2 | 79.5 | 19.0 | 78.0 | 21.1 | 74.8 | 25.0 |
11. Experimenter-selected A duration_V2, f1f2Time50_V2, meanf0_ratio, duration_C3 | 51.2 | 88.2 | 11.6 | 90.6 | 9.2 | 90.6 | 9.2 |
12. Experimenter-selected B duration_V2, f1f2Time50_V2, maxf0_ratio, duration_C3 | 51.2 | 88.2 | 11.6 | 92.1 | 7.9 | 90.6 | 9.2 |
13. Experimenter-selected C duration_V2, f1f2Time50_V2, duration_C3 | 51.2 | 86.6 | 13.2 | 90.6 | 9.2 | 89.8 | 10.0 |
The observed accuracies and balanced error rates of nearly all of the classifiers were statistically significant (p < 0.05), with the best-performing classifier achieving 92.1% accuracy and 7.9% error. The non-significant results came from classifiers using only F0 features and only syntagmatic features.
The classifiers using the full set of 309 features performed well above the baseline (77.2%, 77.2% and 72.4% with SVM-RBF, SVM-linear and LDA, respectively). Classifiers using only the automatically selected set of best F0 features failed to achieve a statistically significant improvement (p < .05 using McNemar’s test) or demonstrated a statistically significant worse performance over the same algorithm using the full set of features. By contrast, classifiers using only the automatically selected set of best non-F0 features achieved a numerical but not statistically significant improvement.
In addition, we observed the following numerical tendencies: Classifiers using only non-F0 measures outperformed classifiers using only F0 measures; and classifiers using only paradigmatic measures outperformed classifiers using only syntagmatic measures. Similarly, classifiers using only the automatically selected set of syntagmatic features failed to achieve a statistically significant improvement or demonstrated a statistically significant worse performance over the same algorithm using the full set of features, while classifiers using only the automatically selected set of best paradigmatic features achieved a numerical but not statistically significant improvement.
The different algorithms performed competitively, and the classifiers using experimenter-selected features achieved the best results overall, all showing a statistically significant improvement over the same algorithm using the full set of features.
5.3 Discussion
The performance of classifiers trained on laboratory-elicited data, like classifiers trained on web-harvested data, strongly supports the theoretical prediction for location of prominence in comparative clauses. With few exceptions, classifiers performed well above the baseline, and satisfied statistical significance (p < .05).
The classifiers trained on laboratory-elicited data also revealed the robustness of classifiers using non-F0 and syntagmatic features. Indeed, classifiers using non-F0 and syntagmatic features achieved statistical significance without exception. Numerically, the classifiers using non-F0 and syntagmatic features also outperformed the classifiers using F0 and paradigmatic features.
The best performing classifiers, however, included a combination of F0, non-F0, syntagmatic, and paradigmatic, whether the features were selected algorithmically with Boruta or whether the features were selected manually by the experimenter. The experimenter selected feature sets included: Duration of I, the first consonant closure duration of did, the F1–F2 differential at the midpoint of I, and the mean or maximum F0 ratio between I and did. The performance of the classifiers which included a syntagmatic F0 measure differed only marginally from the performance of the classifier which lacked either of the syntagmatic F0 measures.
Thus, the performance of classifiers trained on laboratory data confirm that, while undeniably relevant acoustic cues, the F0 and syntagmatic measures are not necessary acoustic cues for these data, contra prosodic-semantic theories of focus according to which F0 (or pitch) and syntagmatic evaluation are unique or pre-eminent. In the following sections, we consider the contribution of individual measures in simpler, logistic regression models of production and perception.
With respect to contrasts between classifiers trained on laboratory data and classifiers trained on web data, we have not shown that there exists a statistically significant difference.12 However, the consistent magnitude of difference, (i) between the web-trained lab-tested classifiers and the lab-tested web-trained classifiers and (ii) between web-trained web-tested classifiers and web-trained lab-tested classifiers, both around 5%, suggests an asymmetry. Both laboratory and web speech may be used as training data for web speech. However, the web speech proved somewhat less effective as training data for laboratory speech.
This asymmetry is consistent with the observation that many of the tokens from the web dataset were produced by professional broadcasters. These speakers are less likely to produce speech that is potentially ambiguous (e.g., produced with coarticulation, reduced vowels) and more likely to mark prosody consistently (Ostendorf & Shattuck-Hufnagel, 1996) and with hyperarticulation. It follows, then, that a classifier trained on clearer speech (with respect to these dimensions) would have more difficulty when applied to laboratory speech than on similarly clear speech; and a classifier trained on laboratory and a classifier trained on laboratory speech would do equally well on speech which is equally clear or less clear.
There is, of course, no a priori reason to expect this difference. Variation in the web speech–for example, in terms of expressivity, recording quality, speaker, or discourse context–might have made it the superior training dataset instead of the laboratory data.
As a consequence, the web data may offer an important source of cross-validation, not only because they are produced more naturally and in a variety of different pragmatic contexts, but because the web speech appears to contain tokens with more idealized realizations. Moreover, it is encouraging that the classifiers work in either direction, as it suggests that results from lab speech lead to valid generalizations that extend to non-lab speech.
Finally, the failure of the base algorithm to identify the good features, and feature selection not doing as well as experimenter selection, is a negative result for a purely automatic procedure. This is a topic for further research using machine learning methodologies. Fresh data and more data will be a help in approaching it.
6 Model comparison of logistic regression
6.1 Results
A direct statistical comparison of two SVM or LDA classifiers would require additional datasets, as discussed earlier. We may, however, get a sense of how individual features are contributing to less complex models, using logistic regression. In this section, we compare a logistic regression model of web1 using experimenter-selected feature set A (duration_V2, duration_C3, f1f2Time50_V2, meanf0_ratio) against four smaller models, each lacking a different member of the feature set, using an Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) (cf. Baayen et al., 2008). ANOVAs for the datasets web1, web2, and lab in Table 5 show that the removal of any one of the four features is statistically significant. In other words, each of these features contributes significantly to explaining variation in the larger model.
web1 | Df | Residual Deviation | Df | Deviance | Pr (>Chisq) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
duration_V2, duration_C3, f1f2Time50_V2, f0_ratio | 85 | 19.266 | |||
all except duration_V2 | 86 | 48.901 | –1 | –29.635 | 5.22E-08* |
all except duration_C3 | 86 | 25.917 | –1 | –6.6512 | 9.91E-03* |
all except f2f2Time50_V2 | 86 | 35.674 | –1 | –16.408 | 5.11E-05* |
all except meanf0_ratio | 86 | 23.917 | –1 | –4.6515 | 0.03103* |
web2 | Df | Residual Deviation | Df | Deviance | Pr (>Chisq) |
duration_V2, duration_C3, f1f2Time50_V2, f0_ratio | 122 | 44.301 | |||
all except duration_V2 | 123 | 80.535 | –1 | –36.234 | 1.75E-09* |
all except duration_C3 | 123 | 60.398 | –1 | –16.097 | 6.02E-05* |
all except f2f2Time50_V2 | 123 | 53.216 | –1 | –8.9146 | 2.83E-03* |
all except meanf0_ratio | 123 | 53.498 | –1 | –9.1969 | 0.00242* |
lab | Df | Residual Deviation | Df | Deviance | Pr (>Chisq) |
duration_V2, duration_C3, | 389 | 208.51 | |||
f1f2Time50_V2, f0_ratio | |||||
all except duration_V2 | 390 | 275.19 | –1 | –66.675 | 3.20E-16* |
all except duration_C3 | 390 | 221.46 | –1 | –12.949 | 3.20E-04* |
all except f2f2Time50_V2 | 390 | 250.28 | –1 | –41.772 | 1.03E-10* |
all except meanf0_ratio | 390 | 216.36 | –1 | –7.8521 | 0.005076* |
6.2 Discussion
The classification experiments revealed the robustness of models with and without syntagmatic F0 features. We observed informally that minimally contrastive feature sets–with and without a syntagmatic F0 feature–did not yield large differences in classifier performance. The logistic regression models tested whether the ratio of F0 means in I and did contributed meaningfully to a model with paradigmatic, non-F0 measures.
Not only did the paradigmatic, non-F0 features each contribute significantly to the logistic regression model, but the ratio of F0 means contributed significantly as well. First, this result supports our conclusion from the classification experiments that both F0 and non-F0 and both syntagmatic and paradigmatic features are relevant to the classification of focus placement in these data. Second, this result is evidence against the hypothesis that the syntagmatic F0 features are redundant in these data (i.e., they contribute additional information). This result does not tell us, however, whether human listeners make use of syntagmatic, F0 information in these data, a question we turn to in the final set of experiments.
7 Human acoustic classifiers
In this section, we assess the validity of the machine learning classifier results by comparing the machine learning classifiers to human classifiers. In other words, we want to know how closely the machine learning classifiers mimic human speech perception in classification accuracy and the acoustic measurements used to make judgements. We conducted two perceptual experiments to answer this question: The first using stimuli from the web dataset; the second using stimuli from the laboratory dataset.
7.1 Experiment 1: web stimuli
7.1.1 Method
A subset of 64 tokens from the web2 corpus dataset was chosen: The first 32 of each semantic focus class. From each soundfile, the sequence “than I did” was extracted to create the stimulus. The files were normalized for sampling frequency and amplitude. The information presented to participants of the perception experiment was limited in this way in order to more closely match the limited information available to the statistical classifiers: Neither machine nor human had the preceding or following acoustic information and neither machine nor human had any linguistic or extra-linguistic context.
Forty individuals participated in the perception experiment, which was conducted at McGill University. Participants were compensated for their time. The data of two participants was not analyzed because the subjects reported making errors. The stimuli were played one at a time, in random order, with no category repeated more than twice. After each stimulus, the listener was asked to complete two tasks: first, to choose whether “I” or “did” had greater prominence; second, to rate confidence in their choice on a scale from 1 (“very confident”) to 5 (“very uncertain”).
Of course, one may question whether a linguistically naïve participant can easily understand what ‘prominent’ means, and whether all participants in this experiment were indeed answering the same question. We note anecdotally, however, that participants seemed to find the task very natural and easy to complete, and given the results we have the impression that participants found the notion of prominence quite intuitive.
We evaluate the results in two ways. First, we calculate accuracy rates and balanced error rates, just as we did for the machine learning classifiers. In this way we can compare the human and machine learning classifiers using the same performance measures. We can also compare these measures by listener and by item. If many listeners consistently misclassified any of the data or any particular items were misclassified consistently, this would suggest a listener or item bias.
Second, we evaluate generalized linear mixed models using two of the top-performing feature sets. Mixed models allow us to incorporate random effects of listener and item. We chose experimenter-selected feature set A (duration_V2, duration_C3, f1f2Time50_V2, mean_f0_ratio) and experimenter-selected feature set C (duration_V2, duration_C3, f1f2Time50_V2), because they were used for most of the top-performing machine learning classifiers, and because they differed in a single feature of interest, namely mean_f0_ratio. The modeling allows us to ask how much variance in listeners’ responses a model using these features predicts. For a given model, we can also ask how predictive the individual features in the model are, and whether the model predicts significantly more variation than another model.
7.1.2 Results
Accuracy/Error: As a group, the 38 participants achieved a mean accuracy of 85.9%, median accuracy of 89.1% and standard deviation of accuracies 8.3%. They achieved a mean BER of 14.1%, median BER of 10.9% and standard deviation of BERs 8.3%. Participants’ individual accuracy rates ranged from 64.1% to 95.3% and their balanced error rates ranged from 4.7% to 35.9% percent.
As for the items used in the experiment, only 3 were consistently misidentified by listeners. The majority of the stimuli were classified correctly more than 80% of the time. The mean by-item accuracy rate was 85.9%, the median 89.5% and the standard deviation 16.9%.
Generalized linear mixed models: In order to understand which acoustic features listeners were using to make their judgments, we tested for the statistical significance of individual features in generalized linear mixed models using the R package lme4 (Bates et al., 2015).
Both statistical models were significant, as were each of the individual fixed effects (i.e., the acoustic features), with the notable exception of mean_f0_ratio (cf. Table 6).
Experimenter-selected A: duration_V2, duration_C3, f1f2Time50_V2, mean_f0_ratio | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Random effects: | ||||
Groups | Variance | Std. Dev. | ||
Participant | 0.066720 | 0.25830 | ||
Item | 0.041699 | 0.20420 | ||
Fixed effects: | ||||
Estimate | Std. Error | z-value | p-value | |
Intercept | 1.249 | 0.5746 | 2.174 | 0.0297* |
duration of I | 36.05 | 2.332 | 15.457 | <2e-16* |
duration of first closure in did | –45.24 | 3.509 | –12.893 | <2e-16* |
F1F2 differential at midpoint of I | –0.003067 | 0.0003291 | –9.318 | <2e-16* |
ratio of mean F0 in I and did | 0.004232 | 0.02654 | –0.159 | 0.873 n.s. |
Experimenter-selected C: duration_V2, duration_C3, f1f2Time50_V2 | ||||
Random effects: | ||||
Groups | Variance | Std. Dev. | ||
Participant | 0.066720 | 0.25830 | ||
Item | 0.041699 | 0.20420 | ||
Fixed effects: | ||||
Estimate | Std. Error | z-value | p-value | |
Intercept | 1.210236 | 0.520745 | 2.324 | 0.0201* |
duration of I | 35.946678 | 2.254716 | 15.943 | <2e-16* |
duration of first closure in did | –45.078265 | 3.401762 | –13.251 | <2e-16* |
F1F2 differential at midpoint of I | –0.003068 | 0.000329 | –9.326 | <2e-16* |
We can quantify whether one of the two models of listener response is more predictive than the other using ANOVA. The various goodness of fit criteria (AIC, BIC and log likelihood) for our two models are very similar and according to the χ2 test statistic, we cannot conclude that the model using feature set Experimenter-selected A predicts significantly more variation than the model using feature set Experimenter-selected C. In other words, we cannot say that adding the feature mean_f0_ratio results in a more predictive model of listeners’ responses.
We can also perform model comparison to assess the contribution of the random effects: Participant and item. A model with participant and item as random effect and Experimenter-selected A as fixed effect explains significantly more variation than a model with participant only as random effect and feature set Experimenter-selected A as fixed effect (χ2 = 80.533, p = 2.2e-16). Similarly, a model with participant and item as random effect and Experimenter-selected A as fixed effect explains significantly more variation than a model with item only as random effect and feature set Experimenter-selected A as fixed effect (χ2 = 21.465, p = 3.603e-16).
Confidence Rating: Participants’ confidence rating turned out to be a very significant predictor of their performance on a given stimulus (generalized linear model: σ = 0.031, z = –10.81, p < 0.001). This indicates that listeners have a degree of introspective access to gradience or ambiguity in the prominence distinction.
7.1.3 Discussion
The performance of listeners in the perception experiment, as measured by classification accuracy and BER, closely matched that of the machine learning classifiers. Recall that the top-performing classifier achieved an accuracy rate of 92.9%: While some listeners’ accuracy rates were as low as 64%, 16 out of the 38 human classifiers achieved an accuracy rate above 90%.13
The comparison of listener response models revealed that item explains a statistically significant amount of listener variation. Review of the item distribution, however, reveals that 3 of the 64 items were effectively outliers, with accuracy rates well below 50%. The poor human classifier performance on these items suggests that misclassification by the machine learning classifiers are likely to be a result of other variation (e.g., speaker disfluency, high signal-to-noise ratio) in the data by which human listeners were equally misled.
One misclassified example, transcribed in (22), received a listener accuracy rate of 18.4%. The co-reference criterion predicts this example will be realized with subject focus since the subjects of the two clauses do not co-refer; however, the matrix clause also has a salient contrast at that time which licenses focus on did.
- (22)
- Growing up at that time and that location, you can’t have more fun as a kid than [I]F [did]F
Example (22) is an infrequently occurring but linguistically possible example of double focus. The task of the machine learning classifiers and the human listeners was binary (two semantic classes for the machine learning classifiers and two prominence choices for the human listeners), while example (22) effectively belongs to a third class.
Finally, the same feature sets used in the top-performing machine learning classifiers (viz. Experimenter-selected A and Experimenter-selected C) were statistically significant in a model of listener response. There was no main effect for the mean F0 ratio feature (i.e., it was not individually significant in the model), and removing the feature did not result in a less explanatory model. This result is consistent with the corresponding machine learning classifiers, for which the addition of the feature mean_f0_ratio did not substantially improve generalization accuracy or error rates.
7.2 Experiment 2: laboratory production stimuli
7.2.1 Methodology
In the second perception experiment, human listeners were presented with excerpts of “than I did” taken from a subset of the laboratory production data.14 The experiment was carried out with the same methodology as in Experiment 1. Forty-one individuals participated.
7.2.2 Results
Accuracy/Error: The human acoustic classifiers performed on par with the machine learning classifiers. For the lab data, the 41 listeners achieved a mean accuracy of 78.5%, median accuracy 81.3%, and standard deviation of accuracies 13.1%. They achieved a mean BER of 13.1%, median BER of 12.2%, and a standard deviation of balanced error rates 6.9%. Participants’ individual accuracy rates ranged from 53.1% to 96.9% and their balanced error rates ranged from 3.7% to 29.3%.
As for the items used in the experiment, 1 lab item was correctly identified at less than 50%. Among lab items, the mean accuracy rate was 78.5%, median 80.5%, and the standard deviation 16.9%.
Stimuli were drawn from eight different speakers in the production experiment. The accuracy rates for individual speakers ranged from 67.4 to 82.0%. The mean accuracy rate among speakers was 74.8%, median 73.9%, and standard deviation 5.0%.
Generalized linear mixed models: As in the first perception experiment, in order to understand which acoustic features listeners were using to make their judgments, we evaluated generalized linear mixed models using two top-performing feature sets. Experimenter-selected A contains the features duration_V2, duration_C3, and f1f2Time50_V2 as fixed effects; Experimenter-selected C contains the features duration_V2, duration_C3, f1f2Time50_V2, and mean_f0_ratio as fixed effects. The two models differed only in the feature mean_f0_ratio. Both models also contained participant and item as random effects.
All of the listener response models were statistically significant. There were main effects for each of the acoustic features, with the notable exception of mean_f0_ratio, which was not significant. The feature duration_V3 was only marginally significant in the lab model using feature set Experimenter-selected A.
Despite a marginal test statistic for the f0_ratio parameter estimate in the lab model (p = 0.23795 is small but above an acceptable rate of α = 0.05), an ANOVA comparing the two lab models with and without mean_f0_ratio suggests that the addition of this feature does indeed result in a more predictive model of listener response (χ2 = 80.533, p = 2.2e-16).
ANOVAs revealed that the addition of item as random effect resulted in a more predictive model of listener response on the lab dataset (χ2 = 13.654, p = 0.0002198). There was not sufficient evidence to conclude that including participant as random effect resulted in a more predictive model.
Confidence Rating: Participants’ confidence rating turned out to be a very significant predictor of their performance on a given stimulus (generalized mixed-effects linear model: σ = 0.05844, z = 7.429, p < 1.10e-13), indicating introspective sensitivity to the reliability of classification judgments.
7.2.3 Discussion
The performance of listeners in the perception experiment, as measured by classification accuracy and BER, was on par with that of the machine learning classifiers.
Both logistic regression models confirmed that the contribution of the paradigmatic, non-F0 measures was statistically significant. There was insufficient evidence that the feature mean_f0_ratio contributed significantly (p = 0.24). However, an ANOVA comparing the two models–one with and one without mean_f0_ratio–revealed that the feature does in fact explain a statistically significant amount of variation in listener response which the other variables in the model do not. This suggests that listeners are using both the syntagmatic, F0 and paradigmatic non-F0 measures in the lab data.
As in the perception experiment using web-harvested data, listeners did have difficulty with a handful of items. The ANOVA comparing models with and without item as a random effect was significant, indicating that item explains a statistically significant amount of variation. The listeners’ less than perfect performance, like the machine classifiers’ performance, may be at least partly explained by these outliers.
8 Conclusion
8.1 Discussion of results
We set out to test predictions of theories of focus interpretation in one constrained environment. According to an anaphoric theory of focus, the location of focus in the comparative clause is determined by the matrix clause. Operatively, the location of prominence can be predicted according to the (co-)reference of the subjects in the main and comparative clauses (cf. 4). The machine learning experiments confirmed the robustness of this generalization with both naturally occurring and experimentally elicited data. Classifiers trained exclusively on acoustic measurements from web-harvested data achieved accuracy rates as high as 92.9% and balanced error rates as low as 6.5% when tested on similar web-harvested data. They achieved accuracy rates as high as 87.6% and error rates as low as 10.5% when tested on laboratory-elicited data, still well above a baseline of 51.0% accuracy. Classifiers trained exclusively on acoustic measurements from laboratory-elicited data achieved accuracy rates as high as 89.0% and balanced error rates as low as 10.5% when tested on web-harvested data.
The human classification experiments confirmed the robustness of the generalization as well. Listeners presented only with web-harvested tokens of “than I did” achieved a mean classification accuracy of 86.4% (standard deviation 8.1%) and a mean BER of 4.5% (standard deviation 2.8%). Listeners presented only with laboratory-elicited tokens of “than I did” achieved a mean accuracy rate of 78.5% (standard deviation 13.1%) and a mean BER of 13.1% (standard deviation 6.8%).
In building the classifiers, we also took the opportunity to compare the contribution of specific groupings of phonetic measures: One division between F0 and non-F0 measures and one division between paradigmatic and syntactic measures. In the machine learning classification, we observed a tendency for classifiers with exclusively non-F0 features to meet or exceed the performance of exclusively F0 features. Of course, we do not wish to suggest that F0 never signals prosodic prominence, as this is well attested. Indeed, classifiers using exclusively F0 measures achieved accuracies as high as 79.5% and it is certainly possible that that improved acoustic modeling of F0 may yield even better classifier performance. We leave detailed examination of the F0 profile for future study but note informally that while many tokens with expected focus on I appeared to have H* pitch accents, we also observed the occurrence of other intonational patterns in which the F0 maximum of I was exceeded by the F0 maximum of did (e.g., Figures 8, 9, 10) or in which the F0 maximum was delayed (e.g., Figure 11). Human listeners may well be able to recover focus despite such variability.
Rather than interpreting the results as indicating non-relevance of F0, we wish to highlight the contribution of non-F0 measures, which turned out to be highly predictive. Non-F0 measures, we argue, offer practical benefits for automatic detection of focus. We also note that many researchers have taken such findings not merely as evidence for the existence of secondary cues of accent, but as evidence against the pre-eminence of pitch accent (e.g., Fant et al., 1991; Heldner, 2003; Heldner et al., 1999; Kochanski, 2006; Sluijter & van Heuven, 1996; among others). Mo (2010) finds that individuals show considerable variation in which combinations of acoustic measures they use to mark prominence and these combinations include F0 to varying degrees.
We propose that the increased duration and especially vowel quality observed in our data correlate with post-lexical or utterance-level stress. Following the autosegmental-metrical tradition, stress is phonologically distinct from pitch accent (e.g., Liberman, 1975; Pierrehumbert, 1980), although related by the requirement for pitch accent to align with utterance-level stress.
For the second grouping of measures, we observed a tendency for classifiers with exclusively paradigmatic measures to meet or exceed the performance of exclusively syntagmatic measures. Again, we emphasize that syntagmatic measures are indeed predictive; classifiers using exclusively syntagmatic measures achieved accuracies as high as 80.3% and it is entirely possible that inclusion of other syntagmatic measures might have yielded even better classifier performance. Rather, we highlight the contribution of the paradigmatic measures, which turned out to be highly predictive.
As described in the Introduction, it is traditionally held that prosodic prominence is relational or syntagmatic, meaning that prominence is processed relative to the sentence that is being uttered (e.g., Hyman, 1978; Jakobson et al., 1952; Ladefoged, 1975; Lehiste, 1970; Trubetzkoy, 1939). This explains, among other phenomena, how a word may be perceived as prominent in either fast or slow speech.
Segmental phenomena such as vowel quality, voice quality and, in some cases, duration are paradigmatic, meaning that they are processed relative to another possible realization. Segmental phenomena, such as the phonological voicing contrast between [p] and [b] are responsible for meaning-distinguishing minimal pairs like pig and big. There are no minimal pairs in English, so the reasoning goes, that are distinguished solely by pitch (e.g., pig with a high tone and pig with a low tone).
Minimal prosodic pairs (or n-tuples) do exist, however, as we’ve seen (cf. 3a, 3b, 3c). How can we understand these essentially paradigmatic contrasts without also denying the syntagmatic character of prosodic prominence? Within metrical stress theory, prosody is hierarchical, and one can speak of prominence at multiple levels. Prominence at the word level is realized phonologically by stress, and it is possible to distinguish individual words using stress (e.g., ímport vs. impórt). Further, one can make intonational contrasts at the phrase or utterance level.
Phonologically, then, the difference between two minimal intonational pairs is thus both syntagmatic—how prosodic elements are grouped and which prosodic element is most prominent within a grouping—and paradigmatic—how the prosodic structure of one utterance differs minimally from the prosodic structure of another.
An important source of evidence against uniquely syntagmatic accounts comes from cases of double focus. Ladd (1991) describes an individual “who used to be able to speak German well but then had then spent a long time living in Sweden and now spoke good Swedish but had trouble with German.” Ladd replies to the individual with (23).
(23) | That’s what happened to my French. |
It used to be good, but then I spent a year in Germany and ended up with good German, and now whenever I want to speak French I get German interference all over the place. |
Semantically, (23) is a case of double focus, on my and on French. And phonologically, this focus is being conveyed with prominence. Ladd observes, however, that prominence on my cannot be purely syntagmatic. It is not the case that my is more prominent than its sister, French; if anything, French is realized with greater prominence than is my. The necessary comparison is paradigmatic: (23) is compared to the minimally different realization in (24).
- (24)
- That’s what happened to my French.
Similarly, measures of prominence on I alone were good unique predictors in the than I did datasets because the salient contrast was not only syntagmatic, but paradigmatic (i.e., between focal and non-focal realizations of I).
(25) | than [I](F) | did | paradigmatic contrast |
↕ | |||
than I | [did]F […] F |
As we’ve noted, the highly predictive paradigmatic measures in our data were also measures of stress, namely duration and vowel quality. We hypothesize that this is largely due to the lexico-syntactic class of the focused constituent (i.e., function words tend to be unaccented unless focused). Ladd’s examples (23, 26) contrast focal and non-focal realizations of the function word my. In our comparatives data, we are contrasting focal and non-focal realizations of the function word I. It is sufficient for the usually non-prominent pronoun to indicate prominence by realizing it with even a low degree of prominence.
It is well known that there are important phonological distinctions between function words and lexical words (e.g., Selkirk, 1996 and references cited therein) and lexical words may require a greater degree of prominence to signal semantic focus. Ladd offers another prosodic minimal pair in which the prosodic contrast is realized on the lexical word butcher. In this well-known example, butcher is understood as an epithet for surgeon when unfocused, and literally as a butcher when focused.
(26) | a. | A: Everything OK after your operation? | |
B: Don’t talk to me about it. | |||
The butcher charged me a thousand bucks! | epithet | ||
b. | A: Everything OK after your operation? | ||
B: The butcher charged me a thousand bucks! | literal |
Ladd intuits that the prosodic contrast in (23–24) is not equivalent to the contrast in (26a–26b). For the pronouns, vowel reduction appears to be sufficient to mark the distinction, while both of the lexical words have unreduced vowels and a contrast in pitch appears to be necessary.
In the than I did datasets, the robustness of measures which are non-intonational and which are extracted only from I reflects the categorical and largely paradigmatic prominence on focused I. A full, unreduced vowel, as indicated phonetically by greater duration and more extreme formant extrema, is sufficient information to identify the function word as focused with considerable accuracy. It is likely the case that humans use a combination of syntagmatic and paradigmatic information, and that the choice is context-dependent. Mo (2010), much like the Boruta feature selection in this study, finds that listeners tend to make use of paradigmatic duration and formant measures, but syntagmatic loudness measures.
The category of focus as it figures in current theory can be characterized as a grammatically mediated correlation between a semantics-pragmatics of contrast and redundancy, and a phonetics of prominence. The positive results obtained here suggest the feasibility of constructing explicit numerical models of this correlation using machine learning, and of testing the predictions of formalized theories of information structure in data collected in the ‘wild’ of spoken language used on the web.
The web methodology that is detailed in Howell and Rooth (2009) retrieves tokens of specific lexical strings. In related work, we collected data for several dozen word-string targets and filtered and transcribed the results (Lutz et al., 2013; Rooth et al., 2013). Structuring datasets around specific target strings is a limitation, but it is also an advantage in that it allows machine learning to use specific features in the target. Investigating the success of the method for other contexts, and generalizing the method to an open-ended class of contexts for focus realization is a topic for future research.
Additional Files
The additional files for this article can be found as follows:
Acoustic measures. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/labphon.8.s1
Stimuli for perception experiment. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/labphon.8.s2
Feature sets selected by Boruta algorithm for lab dataset. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/labphon.8.s3
Notes
- The authors do, of course, also allow for cases of focus within constituents smaller than a proposition. The reader is referred to these works for the full proposals. [^]
- Here the degree variable is existentially quantified. The same results are obtained if there are occurrences of the same free degree variable in the main clause and the comparative clause. [^]
- Although the co-reference criterion divides instances of the comparative exhaustively, it should be noted that there are certain cases in which it does not correspond exactly with theoretical accounts of focus. In particular, the co-reference criterion does not distinguish cases of double focus, such as (i). The co-reference criterion predicts that (i) belongs in class ‘s’ (subject focus).
[^]
(i) You should have earned less last year than [I]F did [this]F year Antecedent: You should have earned x much last year ‘You should have earned x much last year’ entails ‘someone earned x much at some time.’ - The Everyzing and play.it interfaces are no longer available, although the same technology has since been made available for a variety of different content providers, including WNYC, Fox Business, and PBS. For tools which interact with these newest interfaces, readers are invited to contact the authors.
Researchers interested in incorporating web-harvested speech corpora are also advised to identify and review other similar resources appropriate for their needs. At the time of writing, we are aware of at least three additional sources of transcribed and time-indexed naturally-occurring speech available on the web.
Audiosear.ch and its associated API provides full-text search of podcasts including programming from National Public Radio (NPR) and the Canadian Broadcasting Corporation (CBC). Many ASR-generated transcripts are manually corrected and so transcription quality can be quite excellent.
Google and YouTube host videos with closed captioning/subtitles which are time-aligned to short stretches of speech.
Digital artist Sam Levine has provided python scripts (Levine, 2016) which search subtitle files from YouTube and other media or from transcriptions generated by the open-source automatic speech recognition system CMU Sphinx (Huggins-Daines et al., 2006; Lamere et al., 2003).
- As noted in Footnote 4, the audio search landscape has evolved since we first collected our web data and we were limited to 90 and 127 tokens in our training and test sets, respectively. [^]
- The sample size should be 10 times the number of attributes according to Brown and Tinsley (1983), 20 times the number of attributes according to Stevens (2002). [^]
- The methods of regularized discriminant analysis (Friedman, 1989) or shrinkage discriminant analysis (Ahdesmäki & Strimmer, 2010) have been proposed to improve performance of simple discriminant analysis when the number of attributes exceeds the size of the dataset. We do not pursue these methods here. [^]
- The two and three-dimensional models underlying Figures 1 and 2 were obtained with the MATLAB fitcdiscr function. [^]
- The terms ‘feature’ and ‘attribute’ are used here in their statistical or computational sense, referring to a particular vector of data (e.g., the vector of data corresponding to 2nd vowel duration). Note also that the terms ‘feature’ and ‘attribute’ are often used to distinguish predictors before and after kernel mapping, respectively. Since nothing in the study hangs on this distinction, we will use the terms interchangeably. [^]
- This is the typical case; however, as an anonymous reviewer notes, it is not a guaranteed effect. [^]
- Equation for RBF kernel: . [^]
- McNemar’s test does not measure variability due to training set. [^]
- The task of the human and of the machine were similar in that both had access to only the acoustic information from the string “than I did.” Although we are encouraged by the close match in performance, we must also note that it is possible that a human may achieve greater performance on a task more closely related to how they usually use language, as opposed to the metalinguistic task used here of identifying prominence. We leave this for future research. [^]
- We used speech from the first 8 participants of the production study. We used 8 of the original 16 elicited utterances—the same 8 for each of the 8 speakers: Tokens 1, 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13 and 15. [^]
Acknowledgements
We wish to thank audiences at McGill, Cornell, Montclair State, York, and Concordia universities, the 1st Digging into Data conference, Experimental and Theoretical Advances in Prosody (ETAP) conference, the Association for Canadian Studies in the United States (ACSUS), the Montréal-Ottawa-Toronto Phonology Workshop, New Tools and Methods for Very-Large-Scale Phonetics Research, and the Acoustic Society of America. We also thank the RAs in the prosodylab at McGill for their help in running the experiment and processing the results. We are also grateful to Associate Editor Jonathan Barnes and two anonymous reviewers. This research was supported by the following grants: NSF 1035151 RAPID: Harvesting Speech Datasets for Linguistic Research on the Web (Digging into Data Challenge), SSHRC Digging into Data Challenge Grant 869-2009-0004, the SSHRC CRC program, and SSHRC 410-2011-1062 Relative prosodic boundary strength and its role in encoding syntactic structure.
Competing Interests
The authors have no competing interests to declare.
References
M. Ahdesmäki, K. Strimmer, (2010). Feature selection in omics prediction problems using cat scores and false nondiscovery rate control. The Annals of Applied Statistics 4 (1) : 503. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1214/09-AOAS277
R. Baayen, D. Davidson, D. Bates, (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59 (4) : 390. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005
A. R. Barron, (1994). Approximation and estimation bounds for artificial neural networks. Machine Learning 14 (1) : 115. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00993164
C. Bartels, (2004). Acoustic correlates of ‘second occurrence’ focus: Towards an experimental investigation In: H. Kamp, B. H. Partee, Context-dependence in the analysis of linguistic meaning. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 354.
D. Bates, M. Machler, B. Bolker, S. Walker, (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67 (1) : 1. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
D. Beaver, B. Clark, (2008). Sense and sensitivity: How focus determines meaning. Wiley-Blackwell, DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781444304176
D. Beaver, B. Z. Clark, E. Flemming, T. F. Jaeger, M. Wolters, (2007). When semantics meets phonetics: Acoustical studies of second-occurrence focus. Language 83 : 245. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/lan.2007.0053
M. E. Beckman, G. M. Ayers, (1994). Guidelines for ToBI labelling, version 3.0. OSU.(Tutorial and accompanying example utterances available by writing to ).
M. E. Beckman, J. Edwards, (1994). Articulatory evidence for differentiating stress categories In: P. Keating, Papers in laboratory phonology III: Phonological structure and phonetic form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 7.
M. E. Beckman, J. B. Pierrehumbert, (1986). Intonational structure in Japanese and English. Phonology Yearbook 3 : 255. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S095267570000066X
J. B. Bishop, (2008). The effect of position on the realization of second occurrence focus. Proceedings of Interspeech 2008, : 869.
P. Boersma, D. Weenink, (2017). Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.0.29, retrieved 24 May 2017 from http://www.praat.org/.
D. Bolinger, (1958). Stress and information. American Speech 33 : 5. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/453459
D. L. M. Bolinger, (1981). Two kinds of vowels, two kinds of rhythm. Reproduced by the Indiana University Linguistics Club.
B. E. Boser, I. M. Guyon, V. N. Vapnik, (1992). A training algorithm for optimal margin classifiers. Proceedings of the fifth annual workshop on computational learning theory, : 144. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/130385.130401
M. Breen, E. Fedorenko, M. Wagner, E. Gibson, (2010). Acoustic correlates of information structure. Language and Cognitive Processes 25 (7) : 1044. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.504378
M. T. Brown, H. E. Tinsley, (1983). Discriminant analysis. Journal of Leisure Research 15 : 290.
M. T. Brown, L. R. Wicker, (2000). Discriminant analysis In: Handbook of applied multivariate statistics and mathematical modeling. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 209. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-012691360-6/50009-4
G. Bruce, (1977). Swedish word accents in sentence perspective. Lund: Gleerup.
N. Campbell, M. Beckman, (1997). A. Botinis, G. Kouroupetroglou, G. Carayiannis, Stress, prominence, and spectral tilt. Intonation: Theory, models and applications (proceedings of an (ESCA) workshop). September 18–20, 1997, Athens, Greece ESCA and University of Athens Department of Informatics : 67.
C.-C. Chang, C.-J. Lin, (2011). LIBSVM: a library for support vector machines. ACM Transactions on Intelligent Systems and Technology (TIST) 2 (3) : 27. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/1961189.1961199
N. Chomsky, M. Halle, (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.
T. Cho, (2006). Manifestation of prosodic structure in articulation: Evidence fom lip kinematics in English In: L. M. Goldstein, D. H. Whalen, C. T. Best, Laboratory phonology 8: Varieties of phonological competence. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 519.
C. Cortes, V. Vapnik, (1995). Support vector networks. Machine learning 20 (3) : 273. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00994018
N. Cristianini, J. Shawe-Taylor, (2000). An introduction to support vector machines and other kernel-based learning methods. Cambridge University Press, DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511801389
T. G. Dietterich, (1998). Approximate statistical tests for comparing supervised classification learning algorithms. Neural Computation 10 (7) : 1895. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089976698300017197
E. Dimitriadou, K. Hornik, F. Leisch, D. Meyer, A. Weingessel, (2009). e1071: Misc functions of the department of statistics (e1071). TU Wien, 1 pp. 5.
H. B. Drubig, (1994). Island constraints and the syntactic nature of focus and association with focus In: Arbeitspapiere des Sonderforschungsbereichs 340: Sprachtheoretische Grundlagen der Computerlinguistik. Tübingen/Stuttgart: Sonderforschungsbereich, 51 p. 430.
H. B. Drubig, (2003). Toward a typology of focus and focus constructions. Linguistics 41 : 1. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/ling.2003.003
T. Evgeniou, M. Pontil, T. Poggio, (2000). Regularization networks and support vector machines. Advances in Computational Mathematics 13 : 1. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1018946025316
G. Fant, A. Kruckenberg, L. Nord, (1991). Durational correlates of stress in Swedish, French and English. Journal of Phonetics 19 : 351.
J. H. Friedman, (1989). Regularized discriminant analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association 84 : 165. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1989.10478752
D. Fry, (1955). Duration and intensity as physical correlates of linguistic stress. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 27 : 765. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1908022
D. Fry, (1958). Experiments in the perception of stress. Language and Speech 1 (2) : 126.
H. Giegerich, (1985). Metrical phonology and phonological structure: German and English. Cambridge University Press.
L. Gillick, S. J. Cox, (1989). Some statistical issues in the comparison of speech recognition algorithms. International Conference on Acoustics Speech and Signal Processing. : 532. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/icassp.1989.266481
J. A. Goldsmith, (1976). Autosegmental phonology. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. MIT.
K. Gorman, J. Howell, M. Wagner, (2011). Prosodylab-aligner: A tool for forced alignment of laboratory speech. Canadian Acoustics 39 (3) : 192.
G. Grefenstette, (1999). The world wide web as a resource for example based machine translation tasks. Translating and the Computer 21 : 4.
C. Gussenhoven, (1992). Sentence accents and argument structure In: I. Roca, Thematic structure: Its role in grammar. Foris, pp. 79. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/9783110872613.79
C. Gussenhoven, (2004). The phonology of tone and intonation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511616983
M. Halle, J.-R. Vergnaud, (1987). An essay on stress. Cambridge: MIT Press.
M. Halliday, (1967). Intonation and grammar in British English. Mouton: The Hague, DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/9783111357447
B. Hayes, (1981). A metrical theory of stress rules. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. MIT.
M. Heldner, (2003). On the reliability of overall intensity and spectral emphasis as acoustic correlates of focal accents in Swedish. Journal of Phonetics 31 : 39. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(02)00071-2
M. Heldner, E. Strangert, T. Deschamps, (1999). A focus detector using overall intensity and high frequency emphasis In: Proceedings of the international congress of phonetic sciences. San Francisco: Linguistics Department, University of California Berkeley, pp. 1491.
J. Howell, (2011). Second occurence focus and the acoustics of prominence In: C. U. R. Folli, Interfaces in linguistics: New research perspectives. Oxford University Press, pp. 278.
J. Howell, (2016). Replication data for: Acoustic classification of focus: on the web and in the lab 1 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7910/DVN/BGCNPE Harvard Dataverse.
J. Howell, M. Rooth, (2009). Web harvest of minimal intonational pairs In: S. S. I. Alegria, I. Leturia, Proceedings of the fifth web as corpus workshop. San Sebastian, Spain: Elhuyar Fundazioa, pp. 45.
T. Hsing, S. Attoor, E. Dougherty, (2003). Relation between permutation-test p values and classifier error estimates. Machine Learning 52 : 11. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1023985022691
C.-W. Hsu, C.-C. Chang, C.-J. Lin, (2003). A practical guide to support vector classification. National Taiwan University. Retrieved from: https://www.cs.sfu.ca/people/Faculty/teaching/726/spring11/svmguide.pdf.
C. J. Huberty, (2006). Applied MANOVA and discriminant analysis. 2nd ed. Hoboken, N.J.: Wiley-Interscience, DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/047178947X
D. Huggins-Daines, M. Kumar, A. Chan, A. W. Black, M. Ravishankar, A. I. Rudnicky, (2006). Pocketsphinx: A free, real-time continuous speech recognition system for hand-held devices. International Conference on Acoustics Speech and Signal Processing Proceedings. : 1. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/icassp.2006.1659988
L. Hyman, (1978). Tone and/or accent. Elements of tone, stress and intonation 1 : 20.
J. Jacobs, (1999). Informational autonomy In: P. Bosch, R. van der Sandt, Focus: Linguistic, cognitive, and computational perspectives. Cambridge University Press, pp. 56.
R. Jakobson, G. Fant, M. Halle, (1952). Preliminaries to speech analysis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
D. Jensen, (1992). Induction with randomization testing: Decision-oriented analysis of large data sets. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. St. Louis, Missouri: Washington University.
T. Joachims, (1997). A probabalistic analysis of the Rocchio algorithm with TFIDF for text categorization. Proceedings of international conference on machine learning.
K. J. De Jong, (1991). The oral articulation of English stress accent. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Ohio State University.
N. Kadmon, (2001). Formal pragmatics: Semantics, pragmatics, presupposition, and focus. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
S. S. Keerthi, C.-J. Lin, (2003). Asymptotic behaviors of support vector machines with Gaussian kernel. Neural Computation 15 (7) : 1667. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089976603321891855
A. Kilgarriff, (2007). Googleology is bad science. Computational Linguistics 33 (1) : 147. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/coli.2007.33.1.147
W. R. Klecka, (1980). Discriminant analysis. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications, DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781412983938
G. Kochanski, (2006). Prosody beyond fundamental frequency In: S. Sudhoff, Methods in empirical prosody research. Berlin; New York: Walter de Gruyter, pp. 89. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/9783110914641.89
M. B. Kursa, W. R. Rudnicki, (2010). Feature selection with the Boruta package. Journal of Statistical Software 36 : 1. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.18637/jss.v036.i11
P. Lachenbruch, (1975). Discriminant analysis. New York: Hafner Press.
D. R. Ladd, (1991). One word’s strength is another word’s weakness: Integrating syntagmatic and paradigmatic aspects of stress. Proceedings of the seventh eastern states conference on linguistics. 7
P. Ladefoged, (1967). Three areas of experimental phonetics. London: Oxford University Press.
P. Ladefoged, (1975). A course in phonetics. Orlando: Harcourt Brace.
P. Ladefoged, G. Loeb, (2002). Preliminary studies on respiratory activity in speech. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 101 : 50.
P. Lamere, P. Kwok, E. Gouvea, B. Raj, R. Singh, W. Walker, P. Wolf, (2003). The CMU SPHINX-4 speech recognition system. International conference on acoustics, speech and signal processing. Hong Kong : 2.
W. R. Leben, (1973). Suprasegmental phonology. Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
I. Lehiste, (1970). Suprasegmentals. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
S. Levine, (2016). Github repository. VideoGrep, Retrieved from http://antiboredom.github.io/videogrep.
M. Y. Liberman, (1975). The intonational system of English. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. MIT.
M. Y. Liberman, A. S. Prince, (1977). On stress and linguistic rhythm. Linguistic Inquiry 8 : 249.
H. Lin, C. Lin, (2003). A study on sigmoid kernels for SVM and the training of non-PSD kernels by SMO-type methods (Technical Report). Department of Computer Science National Taiwan University.
D. Lutz, P. Cadwallader, M. Rooth, (2013). S. Evert, E. Stemle, P. Rayson, A web application for filtering and annotating web speech data. Proceedings of the 8th Web as Corpus Workshop,
J. Lyons-Weiler, R. Pelikan, H. Zeh, D. Whitcomb, D. Malehorn, W. Bigbee, M. Hauskrecht, (2005). Assessing the statistical significance of the achieved classification error of classifiers constructed using serum peptide profiles, and a prescription for random sampling repeated studies for massive high-throughput genomic and proteomic studies. Cancer Informatics 1 (1) : 53.
Q. McNemar, (1947). Note on the sampling error of the difference between correlated proportions or percentages. Psychometrika 12 (2) : 153. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02295996
A. M. Molinaro, R. Simon, R. M. Pfeiffer, (2005). Prediction error estimation: A comparison of resampling methods. Bioinformatics 21 : 3301. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/bti499
Y. Mo, (2010). Prosody production and perception with conversational speech. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
S. Mukherjee, P. Tamayo, D. Slonim, A. Verri, T. Golub, J. Mesirov, T. Poggio, (1999). Support vector machine classification of microarray data In: AI Memo 1677. Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
T. Naes, B.-H. Mevik, (2001). Understanding the collinearity problem in regresssion and discriminant analysis. Journal of Chemometrics 15 : 413. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cem.676
P.P. Ostendorf, Mari, S. Shattuck-Hufnagel, (1996). Boston University radio speech corpus [Computer software manual] In: Linguistic Data Consortium, Philadelphia:
B. H. Partee, (1991). Topic, focus and quantification In: Proceedings of SALT I. Cornell, Ithaca, NY: CLC Publications, DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/salt.v1i0.2918
J. Pierrehumbert, (1980). The phonology and phonetics of English intonation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. MIT. http://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/16065
A. S. Prince, (1983). Relating to the grid. Linguistic Inquiry 14 : 19.
R Development Core Team (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing, Retrieved from: http://www.R-project.org.
M. Rooth, (1992). A theory of focus interpretation. Natural Language Semantics 1 (1) : 75. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02342617
M. Rooth, (1996). On the interface principles for intonational focus In: T. Galloway, J. Spence, Proceedings of semantics and linguistic theory. Ithaca, NY: Cornell Working Papers in Linguistics (Cornell University), 6 pp. 202. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/salt.v6i0.2767
M. Rooth, (2009). Second occurrence focus and relativized stress F In: C. F. M. Zimmerman, Information structure:theoretical, typological, and experimental perspectives. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 15. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199570959.003.0002
M. Rooth, (2015). Representing focus scoping over new In: T. Bai, D. Özyildiz, Proceedings of the forty-fifth annual meeting of the north east linguistic society. Amherst, MA: GLSA.
M. Rooth, (2016). Alternative semantics In: The Oxford handbook of information structure. Oxford University Press.
M. Rooth, J. Howell, M. Wagner, (2013). Harvesting speech datasets for linguistic research on the web. Final project white paper, Digging into Data Challenge, Retrieved from: http://hdl.handle.net/1813/34477.
B. Sarojini, N. Ramaraj, S. Nickolas, (2009). Enhancing the performance of LIBSVM classifier by kernel f-score feature selection In: Contemporary computing. Springer, pp. 533. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-03547-0_51
F. D. Saussure, (1967[1916]). Cours de linguistique generale. Harrassowitz.
R. Schwarzschild, (1999). Givenness, avoid F and other constraints on the placement of accent. Natural Language Semantics 7 : 141. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1008370902407
E. Selkirk, (1996). The prosodic structure of function words In: J. Morgan, K. Demuth, Signal to syntax. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 187.
E. O. Selkirk, (1984). Phonology and syntax: The relation between sound and structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
E. O. Selkirk, (1995). Sentence prosody: Intonation, stress, and phrasing In: J. A. Goldsmith, Handbook of phonological theory. London: Blackwell, pp. 550.
A. Sluijter, V. van Heuven, (1996). Spectral balance as an acoustic correlate of linguistic stress. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 100 : 2471. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.417955
J. Stevens, (2002). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences. Mahwah, NJ London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
N. Trubetzkoy, (1939). Grundzüge der Phonologie. Travaux du cercle linguistique de Prague 7
A. Turk, S. Nakai, M. Sugahara, (2006). Acoustic segment durations in prosodic research: A practical guide In: S. Sudhoff, Methods in empirical prosody research. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 3 pp. 1. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/9783110914641.1
R. Vanderslice, P. Ladefoged, (1972). Binary suprasegmental features and transformational word-accentuation rules. Language 48 (4) : 819. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/411990
W. Venables, B. Ripley, (2002). Modern applied statistics with S. Springer-Verlag, DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-21706-2
M. Wagner, (2005). Prosody and recursion. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. MIT.
M. Wagner, (2006). Givenness and locality In: M. Gibson, J. Howell, Proceedings of semantics and linguistic theory XVI. Ithaca, NY: CLC Publications, pp. 295. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/salt.v16i0.2938
J. Weston, S. Mukherjee, O. Chapelle, M. Pontil, T. Poggio, V. Vapnik, (2001). Feature selection for svms In: V. T. Todd, K. L. Thomas, G. Dietterich, Advances in neural information processing systems. San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann, pp. 668.
E. Williams, (1997). Blocking and anaphora. Linguistic Inquiry 28 (4) : 577.
S. Winkler, (1996). Focus and secondary predication. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Y. Xu, C. X. Xu, X. Sun, (2004). On the temporal domain of focus. Proceedings of International Conference on Speech Prosody 2004. : 81.